Abstract
Detection dogs can be trained to distinguish the major cystic fibrosis pathogen, P. aeruginosa, from a range of organisms. High sensitivity/specificity support the harnessing of this skill to detection in clinical airway samples. http://bit.ly/31GHl0Y
To the Editor:
The major cause of lung damage in cystic fibrosis (CF) is infection with bacterial pathogens, the most prevalent of which is Pseudomonas aeruginosa, chronically infecting ∼60% patients by adolescence/adulthood (www.cysticfibrosis.org.uk/news/registry-report-2017). P. aeruginosa may be successfully eradicated, but frequently recurs and establishes biofilms resistant to antibiotics/host defences [1]. Chronic P. aeruginosa is closely linked with pulmonary exacerbation frequency, faster lung function decline and earlier mortality [2]. The huge antibiotic burden imposed upon patients and the resulting bacterial resistance, allergies and toxicities compound the detrimental impact of the infection itself. Chronic P. aeruginosa should be avoided if at all possible; early detection and rapid treatment may be crucial in achieving this.
Currently, bacterial infections are detected by culture of airway samples at clinic attendance (2–3 monthly). In the absence of new symptoms, long periods could therefore ensue between cultures. Furthermore, while sputum is most commonly obtained from adults with established bronchiectasis, children and those with milder lung disease rely on cough/throat swabs. These lack specificity and, in some studies, sensitivity, so infections can be missed [3]. More reliable techniques such as sputum induction are time-consuming and expensive, while the gold standard, bronchoalveolar lavage, is invasive so neither technique can be undertaken regularly. Serology has been used with some success, but is not generally performed frequently as accuracy remains somewhat controversial and it requires invasive blood tests [4]. With the improving health of current CF cohorts, obtaining reliable samples from non-sputum producers will become an increasing challenge.
Several teams, including our own, have explored the utility of breath sampling for P. aeruginosa detection. Carroll et al. [5] tested the head-space of culture plates in sealed bags with selected ion flow tube mass spectrometry demonstrating higher levels of hydrogen cyanide (HCN) with P. aeruginosa cultures. The group later reported that the presence of HCN in CF breath lacked the sensitivity to detect early P. aeruginosa infection [6]. We used a similar technique to test a combination of volatile organic compounds in breath, also finding insufficient ability to distinguish infected from non-infected on an individual basis [7]. Within the Strategic Research Centre for Pseudomonas in CF, we continue to explore several of these technologies; however, in parallel, we have explored training the canine nose as a detection system.
Sniffer dogs are familiar in contexts such as airport security, and reports of their use in medical fields are established (diabetes [8]) and emerging (recognising seizure-related auras [9] and detection of cancers [10]). Given the natural odour of P. aeruginosa, we hypothesised that dogs can be trained to detect this organism. One small study, available only in abstract form [11], would suggest this is a fruitful endeavour. As a first step we have assessed the ability of dogs to identify P. aeruginosa from other CF bacteria in culture supernatants.
CF bacterial strains were obtained from the microbiology laboratory of the Royal Brompton Hospital and stored on beads at −80°C prior to culture on agar plates. Each isolate was from an individual patient, but as isolates have not been typed or sequenced, some strains may have been shared. Following overnight culture at 37°C in broth (∼109 CFU·mL−1), they were centrifuged at 3900 xg (15 min) and the supernatant removed and filtered through 0.2 µm disc filter.
Dogs were trained and tested on customised sample presentation stands within which supernatants were presented just below head height under a grill (figure 1a). During training, correct identification of P. aeruginosa samples was rewarded by an auditory click and food reward. Once trained, dogs were presented with P. aeruginosa-positive samples, other bacterial controls or sterile broth in a random, computer-generated sequence. A positive indication (dog stopped or sat down) was rewarded if correct. In the double-blind testing, the indication was entered into an electronic spreadsheet, which immediately revealed the correct identification of the sample, allowing the dog to be rewarded if appropriate. Four blinded studies were undertaken: 1) P. aeruginosa versus other bacteria familiar to the dogs; 2) P. aeruginosa versus previously unencountered bacteria; 3) dilution testing (1:1000 and 1:10 000); and 4) mixed, multi-organism cultures. Exact 95% confidence intervals were calculated for the sensitivity and specificity of each dog in each study, based on their first encounter with each sample. Analyses were undertaken using SAS V9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).
In the first study, P. aeruginosa was tested alongside Staphylococcus aureus, Moraxella catarrhalis and sterile broth (598 samples per dog; three dogs). Mean sensitivity (correct signalling at a P. aeruginosa sample) was 94.2% with a specificity of 98.5% (figure 1b). In the second study, we tested the dogs' ability to identify P. aeruginosa when the controls were bacteria to which they had not been previously exposed: Achromobacter xylosoxidans, Burkholderia cepacia complex, Stenotrophomonas maltophilia and Serratia marcessens (166 samples per dog). Two of the three dogs maintained sensitivity >90%, but in the third it was lower at 62.5%. The dogs were most likely to give a false positive indication at B. cepacia complex (specificity 76.9%). In the third study, two dogs were tested on diluted broths (114 samples per dog). At dilutions of 1:1000 no substantial impact on sensitivity (93.8%) or specificity (94.9%) was seen, but at 1:10 000, sensitivity was lower (56.3%) although specificity was maintained (89.1%). In the fourth and final study, when all four dogs were tested for their ability to detect P. aeruginosa in a mixed culture with either one or two other organisms (Haemophilus influenzae, S. aureus and M. catarrhalis, 104 samples per dog), they still correctly identified P. aeruginosa with a sensitivity of 86.5% and a specificity of 84.1%.
In this series of pilot studies, we have confirmed that, following training, dogs can detect the odour of P. aeruginosa in broth supernatants with high levels of sensitivity, distinguishing them from other bacteria commonly encountered in the CF lung. This ability was generally maintained when the dogs encountered other organisms for the first time, or when P. aeruginosa was present in mixed cultures. Sensitivity declined at dilutions of 1:10 000, equating to ∼105 CFU·mL−1.
The mode of growth of P. aeruginosa in broth is significantly different from that in the CF airway and the volatile signals generated will also probably differ. While it is unlikely training on broth will be sufficient for detection on clinical samples, these studies provided useful proof-of-principle for the concept. Had we been unable to train dogs on this substrate further development into the clinic would probably have been futile. We note the reduced sensitivity of P. aeruginosa detection once broth supernatant was diluted. Although the bacterial burden in the chronically-infected CF airway can be as high as 109 CFU·mL−1, the numbers of organisms in early infection will be much lower. This may pose a limitation to this technique, which we will test at the next stage.
We are often questioned about the clinical utility of this method should it prove successful. While the presence of dogs in our CF clinics might be viewed positively by some of our patients, particularly the children, this is clearly not the way forward. Rather, we aim to develop a non-sputum, non-culture-based test that can be performed by people with CF on a frequent basis. This could involve breath, cough/huff tissues, exhaled breath condensate or even urine. Samples could be collected at home by people with CF and sent to Medical Detection Dogs for screening, a positive indication would then lead to clinical assessment for conventional testing. Once we have optimised the next stage of training, we will perform a direct head-to-head comparison of this method with available “electronic noses” or mass spectrometry-based techniques. Compared with new technologies, dogs may ultimately prove more sensitive or more affordable for screening lower airway infection in CF.
Shareable PDF
Supplementary Material
This one-page PDF can be shared freely online.
Shareable PDF ERJ-00970-2019.Shareable
Footnotes
Conflict of interest: J.C. Davies reports other from Algipharma AS (advisory board and clinical trial lead), other from Bayer AG (UK lead investigator and advisory board), other from Boehringer Ingelheim Pharma GmbH&Co. KG (advisory board), other from Galapagos NV (advisory board, clinical trial leadership), other from ImevaX GmbH (advisory and trial design assistance), other from Nivalis Therapeutics, Inc. (advisory board), other from ProQR Therapeutics III B.V. (advisory board and advice on trial design), other from Proteostasis Therapeutics, Inc. (advisory and clinical trial leadership), other from Raptor Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (advisory board), other from Vertex Pharmaceuticals (Europe) Limited (advisory board and National Co-ord/Global Co-I), other from Enterprise (advisory board), other from Novartis (advisory board), other from Pulmocide (advisory board), other from Flatley (advisory board), other from Nivalis Therapeutics Inc. (advisory board), grants from CF Trust, other from Teva (educational activities), outside the submitted work.
Conflict of interest: E. Alton has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: A. Simbo has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: R. Murphy has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: I. Seth has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: K. Williams has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: M. Somerville has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: L. Jolly has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: S. Morant has nothing to disclose.
Conflict of interest: C. Guest has nothing to disclose.
- Received May 14, 2019.
- Accepted July 2, 2019.
- Copyright ©ERS 2019