Abstract
Sleep disordered breathing (SDB) in children has been associated with inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity, but the associations between SDB severity and the type and severity of behavioural disruption are unclear.
1022 children aged 5–7 years old prospectively underwent sleep studies and behavioural assessments through completion of standardised instruments. Participants were subdivided into four categorical groups based on the apnoea–hypopnoea index (AHI; measured per hour of total sleep time (hTST)), i.e. Group 1: nonsnoring and AHI <1 hTST–1; Group 2: habitual snoring and AHI <1 hTST–1; Group 3: habitual snoring and AHI 1–5 hTST–1; and Group 4: habitual snoring and AHI >5 hTST–1, followed by comparisons of behavioural functioning across the groups.
All 10 behavioural variables differed significantly between Group 1 and all other groups. Post hoc comparisons indicated that Group 2 was the most impaired for most behavioural measures. Furthermore, differences between Group 2 and more severe sleep pathology conditions were rarely significant.
This large community-based paediatric cohort confirms earlier findings highlighting a significant impact of SDB on behavioural regulation, with the greatest impact being already apparent among habitually snoring children. Thus, a likely low asymptote exists regarding SDB behavioural impact, such that further increases in severity do not measurably increase parent-rated difficulties with behavioural regulation relative to controls. Our findings do support the need for considering early intervention, particularly among those children manifesting a behavioural impact of SDB.
Abstract
Snoring in school-aged children, even without sleep/respiratory disturbances, is linked to behavioural disturbances http://ow.ly/G4xJ302JlLF
Introduction
Sleep disordered breathing (SDB) is characterised by recurrent episodes of narrowing or collapse of the upper airway that may lead to intermittent hypoxaemia and hypercapnia, ultimately resulting in sleep fragmentation and nonrestorative sleep [1]. The prevalence of obstructive sleep apnoea (OSA) in children, the more severe form of SDB, ranges between 1 and 5%, but habitual snoring is much more frequent and may affect up to 27% of young and school-aged children [2, 3] with a median of 11–12% being reported worldwide [4–8]. In children, one of the major consequences consistently reported in the presence of SDB consists of the emergence of behavioural disturbances, such as restlessness, aggressive behaviour, excessive daytime sleepiness and poor educational outcomes [9–17]. However, results have been variable across studies, specifically with regard to the patterns of behavioural impact seen at differing levels of SDB. Importantly though, compelling evidence to support a causative association between SDB and hyperactivity and inattentive behaviours in children has emerged in the last two decades [18–23], highlighting a significant relationship between SDB and the pattern of symptoms often leading to a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on the part of paediatricians and child psychiatrists or psychologists. In a recent and the only randomised clinical trial to date, children who were randomised to receive early adenotonsillectomy for SDB demonstrated improvement in behavioural functioning at 7 months follow-up relative to controls [24]. Additionally, daytime sleepiness, hyperactivity and aggressive behaviours have all been documented in children who habitually snore, even in the absence of SDB [25–27].
As would be predicted from a mechanistic association, objectively measured sleep and respiratory disturbances have been found to be relatively frequent among children with ADHD-like behaviours [28–30]. Although the exact mechanisms by which SDB elicits hyperactivity and inattention remain unknown, it is possible that the sleep fragmentation and episodic hypoxia that characterise SDB lead to alterations within the neurochemical substrate of the pre-frontal cortex and its connections with regulatory subcortical structures, with resultant executive and attentional dysfunction [31]. Indeed, snoring children were found to perform poorly on tests aimed at assessing the ability to develop and sustain an organised, future-oriented and flexible approach to problem solving, and alterations in sustained and divided attention have been reported consistently [32, 33]. Furthermore, the manifestations of excessive daytime sleepiness differ in children compared with adults, such that both inattention and hyperactivity constitute significant behavioural correlates of sleepiness in younger patients [34–36].
We should, however, point out that the majority of the published studies have assessed relatively restricted paediatric cohorts, and have shown significant variability across measures and approaches taken with such studies. This has fostered remaining questions regarding both the absolute frequency of behavioural difficulties in children with SDB and the relationship of SDB severity with the magnitude of the symptoms presented in affected children.
Based on the standing literature regarding the potential neurodevelopmental mechanisms underlying the regulatory disruptions observed, we hypothesised that behavioural disturbances would be more prominent in children with any form of SDB, i.e. independent of the spectrum of SDB severity. Through examination of a large cohort of community-recruited children, spanning a wide range of the SDB severity spectrum, we anticipated that challenges in attention and regulation would already be evident at the lower levels of SDB severity, and that greater SDB severity would not yield any incremental behavioural impact.
Methods
Participants
The prospective paediatric cohort included children aged 5–7 years old residing in Louisville, KY, USA (recruited from October 2006 to November 2008) and children residing in Chicago, IL, USA (recruited from May 2009 to October 2014). The study was approved by both the University of Louisville (protocol 474.99) and the University of Chicago (protocol 09-115-B) Human Research Ethics Committees. Informed consent was obtained from the parents or legal caretakers of the children and age-appropriate assent was also obtained from the children themselves.
Questionnaires
Sleep status
Parents of all participating children were invited to complete a detailed and validated questionnaire about their child's sleeping habits as described previously [30, 37, 38]. The information gathered through the questionnaire included sex, age, ethnic background, questions on whether the child had difficulty breathing during sleep, mouth breathing, witnessed apnoea and daytime sleepiness. Snoring and the severity of the snoring were also included. For the question on snoring, the responses were graded as “never,” “rarely” (once per week), “occasionally” (twice per week), “frequently” (three times per week) and “almost always–always” (four or more times per week). Returned questionnaires were scanned into a computerised database and were subdivided according to their snoring patterns into nonsnoring children (responses of never or rarely on snore and not applicable on loudness of snore in questionnaire) or habitually snoring children (responses of frequently or almost always–always (three or more nights per week) on snoring frequency and medium loud to loud on loudness of snoring). Based on the responses, children were able to be identified and effectively grouped into those with and without multiple indices of SDB severity.
Behaviour
Child Behavior Checklist – Revised
The Child Behavior Checklist – Revised (CBCL) is the most commonly used parent report scale for measuring problem behaviours and mood challenges in children. For this study, the original version of the CBCL – Parent Report for youth aged 5–17 years old was used [39]. This version includes eight empirically derived syndrome subscales (Withdrawn, Somatic Complaints, Anxious/Depressed, Social Problems, Thought Problems, Attention-Hyperactivity Problems, Delinquent Behaviour and Aggressive Behaviour), two partial summary scales (Internalising Problems (a combination of the Withdrawn, Somatic Complaints and Anxious/Depressed subscales) and Externalising Problems (a combination of the Delinquent Behaviour and Aggressive Behaviour subscales)) and a Total Problems scale. Test–retest reliability and internal consistency are in the range very good to excellent [40]. The CBCL yields T-scores with a mean±sd of 50±10 for the principle Internalising Problems, Externalising Problems and Total Problems indices that were examined with this study. For the individual scales, the lower end of the T distribution is truncated at 50 (corresponding to the 50th percentile).
Conners’ Parent Rating Scales – Revised
The Conners’ Parent Rating Scales – Revised (CPRS-R) [41] is also a parent report scale, examining the presence and severity of problem behaviours observed in children aged 3–17 years old. It was used in addition to the CBCL because it provides a more specific measure of attention problems in children, which is of particular relevance to this study given the findings that children with SDB frequently meet criteria for a diagnosis of an attentional disorder, more often than unaffected peers. Subscales of the CPRS include Oppositional, Cognitive Problems/Inattention, Hyperactivity, Anxious-Shy, Perfectionism, Social Problems and Psychosomatic ratings. Several summary measures are provided, including an ADHD Index, three Global Indices (Restless-Impulsive, Emotional Lability and Total), and three Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR)-based symptom measures (Inattentive, Hyperactive-Impulsive and Total). Indices are standardised as T-scores, with a mean±sd of 50±10. Test–retest reliability and internal consistency are adequate to excellent [42]. Additionally, a recent meta-analysis [43] supported the utility of both the CBCL and the CPRS for detection of ADHD symptoms.
Overnight polysomnographic studies
All children underwent standard nocturnal polysomnography (PSG) evaluation as described previously [44], with assessment of eight standard electroencephalography channels, bilateral electro-oculography, electromyography, two-lead electrocardiography, oronasal airflow measurement using a thermistor, nasal pressure transducer and end-tidal carbon dioxide, chest and abdominal movement by respiratory inductance plethysmography, and pulse oximetry including pulse waveform using a commercially available data acquisition system (Polysmith; Nihon Kohden America, Foothill Ranch, CA, USA), as described previously [44]. Each of the nocturnal PSG studies were scored as per the 2012 American Association of Sleep Medicine guidelines for the scoring of sleep and associated events by paediatric sleep experts (D. Gozal, M.F. Philby and L. Kheirandish-Gozal) who were blind to the behavioural test results [45]. The proportion of time spent in each stage of sleep was calculated as a percentage of total sleep time (TST). A respiratory event was scored as an obstructive apnoea if it was associated with a >90% decrease in signal amplitude for >90% of the entire event compared with the baseline amplitude, the event lasted for at least two breaths and there was continued or increased respiratory effort throughout the period of the event. A mixed apnoea was scored if there was absent inspiratory effort in the initial part of the event, followed by resumption of inspiratory effort before the end of the event. A central apnoea was scored if respiratory effort was absent throughout the duration of the event, the event lasted for at least two missed breaths and was associated with an arousal/awakening or a ≥3% desaturation. A hypopnoea was scored if the event was associated with a ≥50% fall in amplitude of the nasal pressure transducer, lasted for at least two breaths and was associated with an arousal/awakening or ≥3% desaturation.
Anthropometry
During the nocturnal PSG test, height was measured with a stadiometer and recorded to the nearest 0.1 cm. Weight was recorded to the nearest 0.1 kg. Height and weight centiles were calculated using the Centre for Disease Control 2000 growth charts for the USA (www.cdc.gov/growthcharts). Body mass index (BMI) z-scores were calculated using The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia BMI and z-score calculation in children online software (stokes.chop.edu/web/zscore).
Data analysis
Based on questionnaires and nocturnal PSG findings, children enrolled in the study were subdivided into four categorical groups based on recognised OSA severity levels [46] measured using the apnoea–hypopnoea index (AHI; measured per hour of TST (hTST)), i.e. Group 1: nonsnoring and AHI <1 hTST–1; Group 2: habitual snoring and AHI <1 hTST–1; Group 3: habitual snoring and AHI 1–5 hTST–1; and Group 4: habitual snoring and AHI >5 hTST–1. Comparisons of behavioural functioning across the four snore/AHI groups were conducted using subtests of the CBCL and CPRS-R as described above. We selected 10 behavioural indices from the two questionnaires for comparison across the four groups based on the specific behavioural areas covered by these items; these included the primary indices from the CBCL (Internalising, Externalising, ADHD and Total scores) and the CPRS-R (Cognitive, Hyperactivity, Psychosomatic, ADHD Total, and Inattentive, Hyperactive-Impulsive and Total DSM-IV-TR criteria met). Standardised scores were used for all analyses. Homogeneity of variance across conditions was supported in all comparisons but two, but because of these exceptions, and departures from normality in some behavioural measures, Welch's ANOVA results are reported here. Multivariate procedures were not used due to the amount of missing data across measures, our inclusion of CPRS-R and CBCL scores derived from subscores that were also examined, departures from homogeneity of variance/covariance matrices and our interest in examining all behavioural measures. ANOVA results were determined to be significant at the p<0.05 significance level, although we used Bonferroni's correction to control error rate across all omnibus ANOVAs due to the number of outcome measures. Following significant omnibus ANOVA results, we also used Bonferroni's correction for post hoc tests and adjusted p-values are reported for the results.
Results
From October 2006 to October 2014, 1022 children were recruited from public schools and completed the designated protocol. Specific subject characteristics are detailed in table 1. An initial comparison of mean BMI scores across these groups reached significance, as did proportion of Caucasian and African-American children in each group (p<0.001). Otherwise no significant differences were identified for potential confounders, although sleep variables differed as expected, and are presented here purely for illustrative purposes.
General characteristics of the four groups of children based on the presence or absence of habitual snoring and their polysomnographically derived apnoea–hypopnoea indices (AHIs) (see main text for group definitions)
All 10 behavioural variables differed significantly across snore/AHI levels (tables 2 and 3). The snoring/AHI <1 hTST–1 group (Group 2) suffered from the highest levels of behavioural pathology in all behavioural measures but one. This group was significantly more impaired than the control/nonsnoring group (Group 1) in all measures except CPRS-R Hyperactivity (p=0.078). Group 2 was significantly more impaired than those with greater sleep pathology only for CBCL ADHD (p<0.001 and p=0.041 for Group 3 and 4 comparisons, respectively), CPRS-R Cognitive (p=0.048 for Group 3 comparison) and CPRS-R Hyperactivity (p=0.04 and p=0.037 for Group 3 and 4 comparison, respectively).
Behavioural functioning differences across snore/apnoea–hypopnoea index levels
Behavioural functioning in the four groups of children (see main text for group definitions)
The tendency for Group 2 to demonstrate greater behavioural pathology was consistent across different ethnic groups, asthma status groups and sexes, although insufficient representation in Group 1 precluded further analysis of interaction effects or stratification by ethnicity.
Ethnicity (including only Caucasian and African-American children due to sample size limitations for other groups) was also included as a factor in separate analyses to determine the presence of interactions between ethnicity and AHI group for each behavioural measure. Interactions between AHI and ethnicity were significant for several behavioural measures, although trends were inconsistent and suffered from power limitations due to the small number of African-American children in the sample who did not snore (n=13), and are not the focus of the current paper. Similar analyses using asthma as a second factor revealed interactions for several behavioural variables, although this was primarily due to greater behavioural problems in the asthmatic control group (Group 1), which could be spurious due to the very small sample of nonsnoring asthmatic children (n=13). No such interactions were found for sex. Furthermore, analyses were conducted to examine the differences in behavioural functioning across AHI levels while controlling for age and BMI, which did not suggest any change in trends or significance patterns outlined above, and interactions between these variables and SDB were not significant.
When stratifying by BMI, we found similarity of trends for both normal BMI z-score (<1.24) and high BMI z-score (>1.64) children with regard to greater behavioural pathology for Group 2 than Group 1 children. It appears that for higher BMI z-score children, the pattern of reported behaviour problems remain fairly constant across all snoring groups, i.e. Groups 2–4. However, for children with low BMI z-scores, behaviour problems are more prevalent for Group 2 than for either Groups 3 or 4, although these differences were inconsistent, similar to the overall analysis above.
The trends and significance patterns from stratified analysis results did not differ notably from overall analyses reported here, so they were not included in the current paper. Additionally, BMI and AHI were also treated as continuous covariates in regression analyses during initial screening. However, neither BMI nor BMI×AHI interaction terms were typically significant as predictors of behavioural measures. As a result, these analyses were also excluded from the current paper.
Trends illustrating higher pathology for Group 2 appear fairly consistent across all significant behavioural measures. This generally suggests that across demographic and anthropomorphic conditions Group 2 was associated with a greater number of parent-reported behavioural problems than seen with nonsnoring children, although behavioural trends at higher levels of sleep pathology may be dependent on other factors.
Discussion
This study suggests that SDB during early childhood is a significant contributor to challenges with attention and behavioural regulation, as rated by parents. These findings follow on multiple previous studies, although with much more limited clinical samples [18–27]. Results indicate that behavioural challenges consistent with a potential diagnosis of ADHD are more common in children with SDB, including inattention, motor restlessness and hyperactivity, and associated behavioural dysregulation. Additionally, we found that mood difficulties were significantly noted, highlighting the potential impact of SDB on emotional regulation. However, of greater significance is the current finding that the impact of SDB on behaviour is greatest in children with the mildest degree of severity when compared with controls. This pattern was noted across both a broad-scale assessment, the CBCL, and one that is more directly associated with challenges seen in attentional disorders. Our results suggest that the potential alterations in sleep structure in habitually snoring children that are not readily identified by nocturnal PSG standard scoring approaches may exert effects on the underlying neural mechanisms that support aspects of both directed and sustained attention, as well as frontal-striatal circuits that are known to mediate emotion and behavioural regulation. Of note, the impact of intermittent hypoxia, as derived from the nadir arterial oxygen saturation measured by pulse oximetry (SpO2) levels, was not as prominent, and suggests that the deleterious effects of the latter in the developing central nervous system may be located in different regions that may be more specifically involved in cognition and memory [47, 48]. Analyses identified that snoring and resultant sleep fragmentation also likely serve as the initial important stepwise increase in behavioural impact, such that increased SDB severity in conjunction with higher AHI and lower SpO2 fail to increase the magnitude of behavioural challenge. Although the observed behavioural trajectory across the SDB spectrum does not appear to overlap with the extent of neural substrate injury (i.e. as deduced from the observation that increased SDB severity leads to dose-dependent cognitive impact), it reminds us that a lower threshold regarding the expression of behavioural challenge in affected children may be present. Thus, more severe levels of SDB may in fact contribute to a reduced ability to regulate behaviour in conjunction with an impaired capacity to effectively muster the cognitive resources required to challenge and oppose efforts at external control; this conjectural explanation remains a focus for future research. Still, even in children with higher BMI z-scores, the prominent impact of snoring in the context of AHI <1 hTST–1 remained apparent when compared with higher SDB severity groups, thereby supporting the preferential role of sleep disruption in the processes underlying behavioural dysfunction among snoring children.
Our results were highly consistent across boys and girls. Additionally, initial screening analyses suggested that Group 2 had consistently greater behavioural pathology across all racial groups, but low numbers of children representing some races within defined sleep pathology groups resulted in inconsistencies in significance patterns in the present study. It is worth noting that additional considerations may exist regarding parental sensitivities toward behaviour problems across race and sex that should be considered in future research. Indeed, several studies have shown that parents differ in their ratings of their children's behavioural functioning across both socioeconomic status and race [49, 50]. Additionally, observations regarding behavioural presentation, as well as actual levels of behavioural output, differ in response to health and BMI status. Similarly, parents of boys tend towards stronger negative ratings in terms of attention and behavioural control [51].
An important clinically relevant take-home lesson emanates from these findings: children who snore may be at increased risk for developing and demonstrating attentional and behavioural regulatory concerns, independent of the severity of SDB. Both internalising and externalising challenges were noted to be of significant concern for children with SDB in this study. This highlights a need to address the risk for behavioural disruption in children who develop snoring; the latter is a likely precursor to ongoing challenges with both socialisation and regulation, and could, if unaddressed, adversely impact learning and social adaptation. However, our study does not answer the question concerning whether by addressing the underlying causes of snoring, the anticipation of improved functioning in behavioural control and learning will occur. Recent findings from the only randomised interventional trial for mild to moderate OSA would suggest that the more immediate impact of treatment is reflected in behaviour rather than cognition [24]. Similarly, children presenting clinically for evaluation of behavioural difficulties that may lead, on initial examination, to a consideration of ADHD should be clearly evaluated as to their sleep hygiene and snoring status. Based on current findings, the possibility exists that a significant number of children who are considered for stimulant treatment may actually benefit more directly from sleep assessment and intervention, with improved outcomes. The effectiveness of such interventions, and presence of residual OSA, may additionally depend on factors such as age and BMI [52, 53]. Consequently, the implementation of divergent clinical management approaches for snoring children with behavioural perturbations but otherwise normal sleep studies as compared with similarly snoring children in whom behavioural issues are absent will have to be scrutinised in future research.
In summary, the present study suggests that the presence of snoring, and inferentially of disrupted sleep, underlies the emergence of behavioural perturbations associated with inattention and impulsivity, and that further increases in the severity of SDB are not accompanied by concomitant increases in the severity of the behavioural patterns.
Limitations
One clear limitation to this study is the focus solely on parent reports regarding behavioural functioning, as opposed to assessing a broader environmental impact that would be afforded by additional teacher reports. Given the difficulties that are inherent in obtaining reports regarding behaviour from multiple individuals, a decision was made to rely principally on parents as informers. This decision obviously limits our understanding of the wider presentation of challenging behaviours that may exist for this sample. Furthermore, since both snoring status and behaviour problems were assessed through parent reports, it is possible that parents who suspected an association between snoring and behaviour problems could have introduced some recall bias into these comparisons. However, prior research has also demonstrated elevated behavioural problems in children with sleep disorders using teacher reports [54–57], which are less likely to be biased by knowledge of sleep status. Agreement between parent and teacher reports of behaviour may diverge at high levels of reported behavioural pathology, perhaps due to different environments under observation [57]. In addition, studies have reported high sensitivity and specificity for parent-reported snoring status in relation to PSG [58], and a general lack of parent knowledge of the association between SDB and behavioural problems [59].
An additional salient limitation of the current study is the low representation of nonsnoring African-American children, and the resulting inability to examine possible moderating effects of race on the relationship between SDB severity and behavioural outcomes. Previous research has been much more variable in regard to its sampling, with a focus on clinic-based referrals; the current study, while still showing some variability in the diversity of children included, is one of the largest population-based samples available to date and one that was collected separately from clinic referral. As such, while concerns regarding absolute representation remain, this study does in fact show the risk for impact of SDB within the broader paediatric population. Finally, despite the accumulating evidence of a strong association between SDB and behavioural outcomes, due caution should be exercised in inferring causal inferences from observational research.
Future directions
It will be important in future studies to assess the relationship between behavioural and cognitive variables, such as to potentially uncover a more ecologically based range of impact, and such aim is now a principal focus for future research. Additionally, we believe that integrating teacher reports may increase ecological understanding of behavioural impact and help guide stronger decision making regarding how to best, and when to best, intervene.
Acknowledgements
Authors contributions are as follows: D.L. Smith provided critical input to behaviour function interpretation and data analysis, and drafted the initial version of the manuscript; D. Gozal conceptualised the study, was responsible for sleep study interpretation, performed data analysis and edited the manuscript; S.J. Hunter performed individual behavioural data analysis and contributed to manuscript editing; M.F. Philby recruited subjects, scored and interpreted sleep studies, and participated in database maintenance and analysis; J. Kaylegian participated in sample collection as well as behavioural battery scoring; L. Kheirandish-Gozal conceptualised the study, recruited subjects, scored sleep studies, coordinated the database, analysed data and drafted critical portions of the manuscript; she is also the guarantor of the paper, taking responsibility for the integrity of the work as a whole, from inception to published article. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
Footnotes
Support statement: NIH grants HL-65270 to D. Gozal and HL-130984 to L. Kheirandish-Gozal. Funding information for this article has been deposited with the Open Funder Registry.
Conflict of interest: None declared.
- Received April 23, 2016.
- Accepted July 26, 2016.
- Copyright ©ERS 2016