Abstract
Sleep disruption and daytime sleepiness in obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnoea syndrome (OSAHS) patients result from recurrent apnoeas/hypopnoeas and arousals from sleep. Around 30% of apnoeas/hypopnoeas are not terminated by visible cortical arousals. The current authors tested the hypotheses that arousal induction is linked to sleep stage, oxygen desaturation, event type, event duration and time of occurrence during the night.
Fifteen patients with OSAHS of varying severity were studied and all their apnoeas/hypopnoeas were evaluated.
Eight of 15 patients had apnoeas/hypopnoeas in all sleep stages, and all their 610 apnoeas/hypopnoeas were analysed in the between stages comparison; data from all 15 patients were included in other comparisons. Thirty-four per cent of apnoeas/hypopnoeas during slow wave sleep (SWS) were associated with arousal, significantly less than the 77% during nonrapid eye movement (NREM) 1 and 2 and 62% during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Arousal induction was not affected by oxygen desaturation, event type, duration or time of the night. The apnoea/hypopnoea index was 39·h−1 in NREM 1 and 2, significantly higher compared to 17·h−1 in REM or to 11·h−1 in SWS sleep.
In conclusion, apnoeas/hypopnoeas in slow wave sleep are associated with fewer cortically apparent, visually detected arousals.
K. Dingli was supported by a Research Fellowship from the European Respiratory Society.
Obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnoea syndrome (OSAHS) affects 1–4% of the population 1 and is more common among middle-aged men. The major symptom is daytime sleepiness, a risk factor for accidents 2, 3, which impairs quality of life and creates difficulties and embarrassing situations at work and in the social environment. Previous studies of the factors causing sleepiness in OSAHS patients, have found weak correlations between polysomnographic findings and both objective and subjective measurements of daytime sleepiness 4, 5. In part, this may be because the recurrent arousals, which modelling studies have shown are central to daytime dysfunction 6, 7, are poorly identified 8. Not all apnoeas/hypopnoeas are terminated by visible cortical arousals, which are absent in about 30% of them 9.
However, there may be differences between events which predispose to more marked arousal reactions. These might include event duration and severity of oxygen desaturation. Furthermore, there may be differences between sleep stages in arousability or in the visibility of arousals.
The current authors have therefore investigated the hypotheses that arousability varies between light sleep (nonrapid eye movement (NREM) 1 and 2), slow wave sleep (SWS) and rapid eye movement sleep (REM), and that visible cortical arousals are more common with longer apnoeas/hypopnoeas and with more severe desaturation. The study also aimed to examine the effects of respiratory event type, apnoeas or hypopnoeas, and the time of their occurrence during the night on cortical responses.
Methods
Subjects
Fifteen patients (14 male, age 51 (sd 9) yrs, body mass index 29 (sd 2) kg·m−2) with excessive daytime sleepiness and at least one further OSAHS symptom 10, underwent diagnostic polysomnography. Patients with periodic limb movement disorder, other neurological, cardiac or pulmonary diseases, or intake of medication which affected sleep, were excluded. The study was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee.
Polysomnography
Polysomnography was performed using a computerised recording system consisting of: 1) two central (C3A2, C4A1) and two occipital (O1A2, O2A1) electroencephalogram (EEG) tracings, two outer canthi electrodes (LEOG, REOG) and a submental electromyogram (EMG) electrode; 2) tibial EMG; 3) three-lead electrocardiogram; 4) oronasal airflow detection by a thermistor sensor, thoracoabdominal movement detection by two piezoelectric belts, a digital microphone and a pulse oximeter.
Recordings were carried out between 22:00 and 07:00 h. They did not include continuous recording of supine body position.
Scoring, definitions
Scoring was performed by a single observer, blinded to subject. Sleep and arousals were scored from the C4A1 and C3A2 tracings. Sleep was scored according to the criteria proposed by Rechtschaffen and Kales 11. An arousal was scored when an abrupt shift to a faster EEG frequency (including theta, alpha and/or greater frequencies but no spindles) occurred and lasted 1–15 s, i.e. arousal scoring was based on the definition of the American Sleep Disorders Association (ASDA), modified to the time threshold which was set at 1 s rather than 3 s 12. Arousals with no preceding apnoeas/hypopnoeas were defined as spontaneous arousals. Arousals overlapping or adjacent to apnoeas/hypopnoeas were defined as respiratory arousals. Arousal scoring ranged from 1–15 s, irrespective of sleep recurrence or prolonged awakening subsequent to the arousal; hence, the awakenings as described by Rechtschaffen and Kales 11 were also included in the scoring.
The numbers of each arousal type per hour of sleep were termed the spontaneous arousal index (SAI) and the respiratory arousal index (RAI). Respiratory-arousals were categorised according to the following: 1) 1–3 s of duration, 2) 3–15 s of duration, or 3) Rechtschaffen and Kales 11 awakenings >15 s. The respiratory events which were not terminated by cortical arousals were categorised into events causing: 1) no EEG changes, or 2) lightening in sleep stage in the epoch following the event termination (e.g. from stage 3 to 2) (fig. 1⇓).
Four examples of scoring: a) hypopnoea inducing a 5-s arousal; b) hypopnoea inducing a 2-s arousal; c) apnoea causing awakening; and d) apnoea that begins in rapid eye movement (REM)-sleep and self-terminates in the following epoch, causing a transition from REM to stage 1 (lightening). EOG: electrooculogram; EEG: electroencephalogram (tracings: C3A2, C4A1, O1A2 and O2A1); EMG: electromyogram; ECG: electrocardiogram; O2: oxygen.
Apnoeas were defined as cessation of the oronasal airflow, lasting ≥10s. Hypopnoeas were defined as airflow reduction of >50%, compared to a 10 s peak amplitude during the preceding 2 min, lasting ≥10 sec and associated with either oxygen desaturation of ≥3% or an arousal 13.
Arousals and respiratory events were scored separately during two sessions.
Oxygen saturation was based on a second-by-second measurement within a window set from the beginning of each event until the middle of the following and/or a maximum of 120 s.
Apnoea/hypopnoea index (AHI) and arousal indices were calculated for each sleep state.
To assess the accuracy and reproducibility of scoring outcomes, the polysomnograms from the 15 patients were scored twice by the same researcher. In these intrarater analyses the researcher was blind to the patient details and the results of the comparison studies. Intrarater comparisons were made 12 months after the initial study was scored. Variability was calculated as the difference between the two scores in per cent. Across the patients the highest variability between scores was 3% for arousal index, but 94% of these scores differed by <3 arousals·h−1 (table 1⇓).
Intrarater variability for sleep states, sleep efficiency, apnoea-hypopnoea index (AHI) and arousal index (ArI) in 15 patients
The influence of the following factors on the induction of respiratory arousals were then investigated: 1) sleep state during which the respiratory event occurred, 2) event type (apnoea versus hypopnoea), 3) event duration, 4) oxygen desaturation, and 5) time of night.
Statistical analysis
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed for the detection of sleep state-related differences in AHI, RAI and SAI, across the patients. Post-hoc analysis was performed with the least-significant difference test for pairwise multiple comparisons.
ANOVA was performed to detect changes in the induction of respiratory arousals due to the time of the night. Paired comparisons between the RAI before and after 02:30 h during NREM 1 and 2 were performed to further assess changes in arousal threshold.
The Chi-squared test was performed to assess significant association between respiratory event type and arousal induction. Paired t-tests were performed to evaluate the significance of event duration and oxygen desaturation on the arousal induction.
Tests were two-tailed and p<0.05 was accepted as statistically significant. The data are presented as the standard error of the means (sem) or the interquartile range (IQR).
Results
Sleep architecture
All patients showed a disturbed sleep pattern. Mean sleep efficiency was 87.6±3.5% of the sleep period time. Mean time spent in NREM 1 and 2 was 64.4±3.6%, in SWS 21.2±2.4%, and in REM 14.3±2.2% of the total sleep time.
Apnoeas/hypopnoeas and cortical arousals
Across the 15 polysomnograms, 2,667 apnoeas/hypopnoeas and 2,056 respiratory arousals were scored (fig. 2⇓). There were 2,326 apnoeas/hypopnoeas during NREM 1 and 2, 128 during SWS and 213 during REM sleep. There were 1,900 respiratory arousals during NREM 1 and 2, 31 during SWS and 125 during REM sleep.
Of all apnoeas/hypopnoeas 67.4% caused American Sleep Disorders Association arousals (□), 4.0% caused 1–3-s arousals (┘), 5.9% caused awakenings described by Rechtschaffen and Kales (11] (), 22.0% were terminated without any visible electroencephalographic change (
) and 0.7% (▪) caused sleep lightening during the epoch that followed the event termination.
The mean AHI of the 15 patients was 31·h−1 (range 10–75·h−1). The mean arousal-index was 34·h−1 (range 16–70·h−1). The mean SAI was 10·h−1 (range 4–25·h−1) and mean RAI was 24·h−1 (range 8–52·h−1). Eighty per cent of the respiratory events caused visible cortical arousals, 20% did not (table 2, individual data table 3⇓).
Apnoea-hypopnoea index (AHI) and arousal indices in different sleep stages
Apnoea/hypopnoea index, respiratory arousal index, spontaneous arousal index and sleep states
AHI differed significantly between sleep stages across the 15 patients (p<0.001). Pairwise comparisons showed significantly higher AHI during NREM 1 and 2 compared to SWS (p<0.001) and to REM sleep (p=0.001), but no significant differences between REM and SWS (p=0.4).
ANOVA, with the percentage of arousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas in the three sleep states being the within-subject variable, was performed between patients whose AHI was greater than zero in all three sleep states. Seven of the 15 patients were excluded as they failed to meet this criterion (table 3⇓). All data from the remaining eight patients including all 610 apnoeas/hypopnoeas showed significant, sleep stage-related differences in arousal induction (F=7.0, p=0.005). Pairwise comparison showed a lower percentage of arousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas during SWS compared to NREM 1 and 2 (p=0.001) and REM (p=0.02). There was no significant difference between NREM 1 and 2 and REM (p=0.2) (fig. 3⇓).
Comparison of arousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas during slow wave sleep (SWS), rapid eye movement (REM) and nonrapid eye movement (NREM) 1 and 2 sleep. ┘: % respiratory events with arousal; □: % respiratory events without arousal. Per hour, REM: 62±8% apopnoeas/hypopnoeas caused arousals; NREM: 77±6% caused arousals; SWS: 34±8% caused arousals across n=8 patients.
Individual apnoea/hypopnoea index (AHI) and arousal indices in each sleep stage
The SAI differed significantly between the three sleep-states, across the 15 patients (F=14.9, p<0.001). Pairwise comparison showed significant differences between NREM 1 and 2 and SWS (p<0.001), NREM 1 and 2 and REM (p<0.001), but not between SWS and REM (p=0.7).
Apnoeas versus hypopnoeas
Of all the 2,667 respiratory events, 1,430 were hypopnoeas and 1,237 were apnoeas. During NREM 1 and 2 53% were hypopnoeas, during SWS 88% and during REM sleep 43%. Seventy-five per cent of hypopnoeas and 80% of apnoeas induced arousals. Chi-squared testing showed no significant difference between the respiratory event type (apnoea or hypopnoea) in the arousal induction either overall or in any sleep-state across the patients (p>0.1) (table 4⇓).
Absence of association between respiratory event type and arousal induction#
Apnoea/hypopnoea duration, oxygen desaturation and arousals
The median duration of arousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas was 22.5 s (IQR 17–30 s), the median duration of nonarousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas was 20 s (IQR 15.5–20 s). Median oxygen desaturation of arousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas was 5.0% (IQR 0–7.0%), and of nonarousal-inducing events was 5.0% (IQR 4.0–7.0%). Across the 15 patients, comparisons of median duration and oxygen desaturation between arousal-inducing versus nonarousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas were not significant overall (p=0.8, p=0.07) or during NREM 1 and 2 (p=0.9, p=0.2). In the eight patients in whom apnoeas/hypopnoeas occurred in all three sleep-states, no significant differences were found in REM (p=0.3, p=0.6) or SWS (p=0.3, p=0.2) (table 5⇓).
Duration of hypopnoeas and apnoeas and oxygen desaturation
Time of the night
Time of the night did not influence the cortical response to apnoeas/hypopnoeas across the 15 patients (p=0.2) (fig. 4⇓). Paired comparison between the mean RAI before and after 02:30 h during NREM 1 and 2, showed no significant changes across the 15 patients (p=0.6).
Hourly comparison of the respiratory arousal index (RAI) between 00:00 and 05:00 h showed no significant differences in apnoea/hypopnoea-related arousability during the night, across the 15 patients (p=0.2).
Discussion
This study has shown that apnoeas and hypopnoeas are less frequent in REM and SWS than light sleep. Arousals caused by apnoeas/hypopnoeas are less common from SWS, while spontaneous arousals are less common from SWS and REM than from light sleep. Furthermore, the cortical arousal response was not influenced by the event type, duration, desaturation or time of occurrence during the night.
Arousal threshold
This study confirms the variability in the arousability of OSAHS patients in response to apnoeas/hypopnoeas during SWS, compared to light and REM sleep. This is in agreement with the findings of Berry et al. 14 who, based on measurements of respiratory effort, observed a cyclical increase in cortical arousal-threshold across the night, parallel to the increase of delta power.
The frequency of spontaneous arousals also changed significantly during SWS and REM sleep compared to light sleep, suggesting a higher arousal threshold during these sleep states. The difference between respiratory and spontaneous arousal frequencies during REM sleep might relate to the loss of muscle tone and the higher degree of upper airway instability 15. This makes airway collapse more likely in REM sleep. Once started, the events tend to end with arousal often after long events, whereas spontaneous arousals are less common in REM sleep. Furthermore, some “spontaneous” arousals may partly be linked to undetected, increased ventilatory effort 14, 16.
The current authors hypothesise that all afferent impulses reach the thalamic/hypothalamic region but onward transmission to the cortex is dependent on the sleep state. In SWS this transmission is inhibited, resulting in decreased detection of cortical arousals but maintenance of “nonvisible” autonomic arousals 17. This hypothesis is supported by Braun et al. 18 who demonstrated changes in cerebral blood flow detected by positron emission tomography throughout the sleep-wake cycle, with selective deactivation of fronto-parietal areas and their functional disconnection to thalamic nuclei during SWS. Activity in primary and secondary sensory cortices was preserved for arousal from deep sleep in response to sudden threat. This finding suggests that the homeostatic drive for sleep, which cumulatively increases in sleep apnoeics as a result of repetitive arousing stimuli within and between nights 19, may be more prominent during SWS.
Apnoeas versus hypopnoeas, arousals and apnoea/hypopnoea index
The present study could not confirm the findings of Stradling et al. 20 regarding the increased cortical response to apnoeas compared to hypopnoeas. This is in accordance with the current authors' clinical impression that patients with predominately apnoeic events are not sleepier than patients with predominately hypopnoeic events. Stradling et al. 20 used a different hypopnoea definition, based on a 50% thermal airflow reduction but with no desaturation or visible arousal requirement. The latter study examined a mean of 18 apnoeas/hypopnoeas in 16 patients, albeit in carefully selected situations, in comparison to the mean of 178 events in 15 patients in the present study. The significant electroencephalographic differences at apnoea versus hypopnoea termination were not detected through the visual scoring which used a 3 s-threshold for arousal scoring, but through neural network analysis.
The sensitivity of the thermistor for the detection of hypopnoea may be questioned as it is a good qualitative but relatively poor quantitative sensor of airflow changes 21. However, it is widely used. To avoid hypopnoea over-scoring, arousal and/or ≥3% oxygen desaturation were added to the 50% amplitude reduction requirement, as recommended by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine 13. The ratio of arousal-inducing hypopnoeas found in the present study is similar to that found by Gould et al. 21 although the method of hypopnoea detection and the scoring criteria differed. It is therefore possible, but unlikely, that the presently used hypopnoea definition has contributed to the lack of difference between apnoeas and hypopnoeas in inducing arousals.
The ratio of apnoeas to hypopnoeas varied with sleep stage, the ratio varying broadly in line with the AHI in that sleep stage. In SWS both the AHI and ratio of apnoeas to hypopnoeas were lowest. Both presumably reflect a greater relative stability of the upper airway in SWS compared with the other sleep stages. Given that there was no difference between arousal frequency following apnoeas compared with hypopnoeas overall, nor in any sleep stage, it is unlikely that the difference in arousal frequency in SWS resulted from this difference in event type.
The AHI decreased significantly from light to REM to SWS in the present study. Previous reports showed higher AHI during REM compared to NREM sleep 22. This difference to the present outcomes maybe due to: 1) previous comparisons of REM against NREM stages 1–4, and/or 2) the fact that the presently studied group spent significantly more time in NREM 1 and 2 and 2,326 of the 2,667 apnoeas + hypopnoeas occurred during this state.
Event duration, oxygen saturation and time of the night
The significance of apnoea/hypopnoea duration and level of oxygen desaturation on the arousal induction was not consistent across the 15 patients.
The nonsignificant relation to oxygen desaturation may be due to: 1) poor accuracy of the method to reflect chemoreceptor activation, and/or 2) its nonsignificance in arousal induction, as previously suggested 23. Hence, oximetry alone cannot reflect the extent of sleep fragmentation, suggesting that it is not a good diagnostic tool.
The lack of association between apnoea/hypopnoea duration and arousal induction is in accordance with previous observations of lengthening of apnoea across the night 24, as it implies that the more sleep is disrupted, the higher the arousal threshold becomes and the less important apnoea/hypopnoea duration becomes in inducing arousal, which is the case in the patient group studied.
The present study could not confirm the findings of a modelling study regarding the progressive increase in arousal threshold across the night 7. The current findings are in keeping with the findings of Rees et al. 9 and they may be due to the disturbed sleep cycle and the within and between nights sleep deprivation in sleep apnoea patients, whose circadian rhythm and the homeostatic sleep dynamics are disturbed. The result is a high arousal threshold at sleep onset in this group of patients.
Study limitations
Limitations of this study include power, definitions and multiple comparisons. Fifteen patients were studied, but seven of these were excluded post hoc from the across sleep-states comparisons as they had no events in a sleep stage, usually in SWS and for three patients in REM sleep. Therefore, it can be questioned whether these data are applicable to all OSAHS patients. The findings between the eight studied patients are consistent in regard to the low ratio of arousal-inducing apnoeas/hypopnoeas during SWS, which varied between 0–66% (table 3⇑).
The patients were randomly selected and the group studied included mild, moderate and severe sleep apnoeics with symptoms and polysomnographic findings typical of the syndrome. The low percentage of REM and SWS is common in untreated OSAHS patients and is an indicator of treatment success and compliance 25.
All the recorded apnoeas/hypopnoeas were studied. This represents a large body of data with which to answer the questions posed. The current results are not only robust, but represent the largest body of events which have been similarly analysed. Therefore, the study is adequately powered to detect clinically important differences.
There are many definitions of hypopnoea and arousal used in the literature. The hypopnoea definition used here fulfils the ASDA recommendations 13. The inclusion of arousals or desaturation in the definition may lead to a greater association with arousals than would be found using other ASDA accepted definitions, i.e. based on thoraco-abdominal movement or nasal pressure alone. The arousal definition used, has been used in previous studies and validated against respiratory events and outcomes 9.
The current authors tried to keep the number of comparisons to a minimum and the study defined primary end points clearly. However, the present study involved 17 statistical comparisons which would bias the study to show significant differences, whereas the current results are largely negative, including the a priori hypotheses that event type, duration, desaturation and time of the night would predispose to arousal induction.
The current authors acknowledge that the present study did not assess all factors which may influence the cortical response to apnoeas/hypopnoeas. These include changes in upper airway resistance, snoring and body posture. Stradling et al. 20 have shown that the latter is not a significant contributor to cortical changes, as detected through neural networks. However, analysis of these factors in the present study was not planned and is not possible retrospectively from the data collected.
Despite the modification in the time threshold used to score arousals, 20% of respiratory events were not terminated by cortical arousals in the present study. This suggests that lack of detection of cortical changes at apnoea/hypopnoea termination is not due to the arousal definitions used but is related to sleep/wake physiology, the processes of which can not be detected through the standard techniques.
It is unclear what terminates apnoeas/hypopnoeas which are not terminated by visible cortical arousals. One possible explanation is that a different type of reaction, the autonomic, terminates apnoeas/hypopnoeas at thalamus/hypothalamus level, linked to presso-/mechano-/chemo-receptor afferent stimuli 8, 16. Autonomic reactions are not represented at the cortex, possibly due to the higher cortical arousal threshold, which during SWS is linked to the synchronised appearance of delta waves. Alternatively, such autonomic arousals may be represented at the cortex but the changes cannot be detected with standard techniques and/or topology.
Monitoring of frontal brain activity as well as autonomic activity and/or the application of neural networks or Fast Fourier Transform analysis might improve the detection of changes at apnoea/hypopnoea termination and thus minimise the difference between apnoea/hypopnoea index and respiratory arousal index. There is some evidence in support of this contention 20, 26, 27, 28. Whether differences between sleep stages would then be requires further study.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank N. McArdle, P. Wraith and H. Engleman for their helpful comments, E. Dolan for her secretarial assistance.
- Received July 19, 2001.
- Accepted April 11, 2002.
- © ERS Journals Ltd